What is the one tool that we use to solve problems and
advance culture through the retention and transmission of knowledge? Exactly,
it is language. What happens when we first learn a language? Early in life we
appear to be able to differentiate between virtually all phonetic units in the
world languages (Kuhl et al., 2008). And around the age of nine months infants
adjust to the information of their native language. For example,
cross-sectional and longitudinal studies showed that while 6-8 months old
English infants distinguished between non-native consonant contrasts easily
such as in the Hindi language, 10-12 months old infants had difficulty with
this task (Werker & Tees, 1984a; Werker & Lalonde, 1988). Similarly,
infants’ ability to distinguish between non-native vowel categories was
reported to decrease as well (Werker & Tees, 1984a; Werker & Lalonde,
1988).
Thus, we can establish that our speech perception abilities
undergo a binary modification towards the end of the first year of life (Kuhl
et al., 2008). At around the same time in the English language, mothers talk to
their infants in a way that has come to be known as motherese or
infant-directed speech (IDS). Infant-directed speech involves acoustic changes
such as increased mean pitch, increased emotional affect, and slow speech rate.
Mothers also exaggerate the differences in the sound of vowels: Thus, if you
have the words ‘sheep’, ‘shoe’ and ‘shark’, you will make the vowels ‘ee’, ‘oo’
and ‘aa’ sound more different from one another. This is known as increased
vowel space (Kuhl, Andruski, Chistovich, Chistovich, & Kozhevnikova, 1997; Uther,
Knoll, & Burnham, 2007).
All these acoustic changes are considered to facilitate
language acquisition in infants. Prior research that compared IDS to clear read
speech to test if those changes in IDS are uniquely didactic did indeed
demonstrate increased vowel space in IDS compared to clear read speech
(Kangatharan, 2016). Exaggerated vowel space has previously been correlated to
speech intelligibility in IDS (Bradlow, Torretta, & Pisoni, 1996). It is
exactly this acoustic aspect that was also observed in speech to adult
foreigners (Uther et al., 2007).
The observation of exaggerated vowel space in IDS and in
foreigner-directed speech (FDS) can be understood in the context of the
Hyper-HypoSpeech theory by Lindblom (1990) according to which any changes in
your speech signal are considerably influenced by who the listener is and what
the speaking environment is. It is considered that speech production is a
listener-oriented modification of speech. In this regard, the observation of
exaggerated vowel space in IDS and FDS can be explained in the English language
by the fact that both groups have the same linguistic needs (Uther et al.,
2007). Thus, one can categorize these two groups as language learners as they
are in clear need of an atypically clearer speech input, and speech with exaggerated
vowel space is supposed to maximize speech intelligibility.
Recent research specifically found exaggerated
vowel space to occur in speech to foreign-sounding rather than foreign-looking
listeners (Kangatharan, 2016). This confirms in line with the H&H theory
that speech is modified with regard to the listener’s linguistic needs rather
than visual appearance. It was also demonstrated that speech with expanded
vowel space resulted in increased speech intelligibility in both native English
speakers and in second language learners of English in contrast to speech
without expanded vowel space. The findings indicate that both first and second
language learners of English can perceptually benefit from verbal adaptions in
the English language if they have a didactic function (Kangatharan, 2016).
References
Bradlow,
A. R., Torretta, G. M., & Pisoni, D. B. (1996). Intelligibility of normal
speech. I. global and fine-rained acoustic-phonetic talker characteristics. Speech
Communication, 20, 255–272.
Kangatharan,
J. (2016). “The role of vowel hyperarticulation in clear speech to foreigners
and infants: PhD Thesis publication.” European
Acoustics Association Young Acoustics Network Newsletter, 47, Issue 2, 4-5.
Kuhl,
P.K., Andruski, J.E., Chistovich, I.A., Chistovich, L.A., Kozhevnikova, E.V.,
Ryskina, V.L., Stolyarova, E.I., Sundberg, U., & Lacerda, F. (1997).
Cross-language analysis of phonetic units in language addressed to infants. Science,
277, 684–686.
Kuhl, P. K.,
Conboy, B. T., Coffey-Corina, S., Padden, D., Rivera-Gaxiola, M., & Nelson,
T. (2008). Phonetic learning as a pathway to language: New data and native
language magnet theory expanded (NLM-e). Philosophical Transactions of the
Royal Society B, 363, 979-1000.
Lindblom,
B. (1990). ‘Explaining phonetic variation: A sketch of the H&H theory’,
403-439 in Hardcastle W J & Marchal A (eds): Speech Production and
Speech Modeling, Dordrecht:Kluwer.
Uther,
M., Knoll, M. A., & Burnham, D. (2007). Do you speak E-NG-L-I-SH? A
comparison of foreigner- and infant-directed speech. Speech Communication,
49, 2-7.
Werker, J.
F., & Tees, R. C. (. (1984a). Cross-language speech perception: Evidence
for perceptual reorganization during the first year of life. Infant Behavior
and Development, 7, 49-63.
Werker, J.
F., & Lalonde, C. E. (1988). Developmental Psychology, 24, 672-683.
No comments:
Post a Comment